Skip to main content

International 3Rs Prize now open for applications. £30k prize (£2k personal award) for outstanding science with demonstrable 3Rs impacts.

NC3Rs | 20 Years: Pioneering Better Science
Guidance

Housing and husbandry: Mouse

Information to help refine the housing and husbandry of the laboratory mouse.

Introduction

The laboratory mouse, derived from the wild mouse (Mus musculus), is a predominantly nocturnal, highly social animal, living in territories populated by family groups. Although partially adapted to captive life, laboratory mice show similarities to their wild cousins and will build nests, burrow, climb and forage if given the chance (Figure 1). Providing them with a living environment that allows them to perform their natural behaviours will improve their welfare.

Black laboratory mouse in its nest surrounded by bedding, you can see the mouses upper head, ears and nose
Figure 1. Laboratory mouse in its nest.

Behaviour

As nocturnal animals, mice are mostly active from dawn to dusk and retreat to shelters during the daytime. During their active hours, apart from eating, which they do in short and frequent bouts, they spend their time exploring their territories, running, climbing and digging. Mice have poor eyesight; however, they have good peripheral vision which allows them to detect movement and they use their whiskers and bodies to sense their surroundings. For their daily activities, they rely heavily on their sense of smell and acute hearing. Mouse hearing extends into the ultrasonic frequencies and ranges from one kHz to about 100 kHz whereas the human hearing range is between 20 Hz and 20 kHz [1]. As prey animals, mice will generally avoid open spaces and instead prefer to move along the edge of features in their environment (thigmotaxis). Being curious by nature, they will explore their surroundings and memorize pathways and elements in their habitats, such as food sources, shelter and obstacles.

Mice emit a variety of ultrasonic vocalizations (USVs), in a wide frequency range of 20–100 Hz, which they use as communication signals in different social contexts, such as parental care, during courtship or during territorial and aggressive behaviours [2,3]. So far, and contrary to rats, USVs in mice have not been shown to correlate with negative or positive emotions; instead their main purpose seems to be the facilitation or inhibition of social interactions. However, recently there has been a lot of interest to systemically study and categorise mouse USVs [4,5] in order to investigate whether they can be used to assess welfare.

Laboratory mouse strains show considerable differences in the expression and intensity of their behaviours [6], and these differences are thought to be genetically determined and dependent on their origins. Nest building is a good example of such behavioural differences between strains, with BALB/c mice being better nest builders than other inbred strains, like C57Bl/6 and CD-1 [7,8] (Figure 2). Similarly, there are considerable differences between strains in preference for enrichment items [9,10]. Levels of aggression are also thought to vary by strain, and several investigators have reported that DBA/2 mice are more aggressive than C57BL/6 [6][11].

C57BL/6 mice build cup-shaped nests
Figure 2. C57BL/6 mice build cup-shaped nests. Image courtesy of Cardiff University.

Housing

Housing and husbandry conditions can have a major impact on laboratory mice throughout their lives. The physical and social environment in which mice are housed has important consequences both for their welfare and the quality of research data derived from them [12,13]. Substandard housing conditions can lead to abnormal behaviours such as increased aggression, stereotypies and barbering, with some strains being more prone to developing those behaviours [14,15]. One aspect of housing that is relatively neglected, is the preference of mice to nest away from their urine and faeces. Standard laboratory housing doesn’t allow the mice to spatially segregate their daily activities in this way and may therefore negatively impact their welfare [16]. Another example of an apparently inconsequential housing variable is the colour of the cage, which could influence the levels of anxiety experienced by the mice and increase their attempts to flee from their cage and their handlers. Mice tend to prefer opaque cages and seem to breed better in them [17,18], nonetheless, transparent plastic cages have become the norm in recent years. Transparent cages allow observation of the animals without the need to remove them from cage or rack, which could potentially cause stress. It is clear, therefore, that there are a number of factors that need to be considered for optimal housing and husbandry of laboratory mice.

Enclosures

Minimum enclosure sizes for mice in European research establishments are fully detailed in Table 1.1 of Annex III to Directive 2010/63/EU. The minimum permitted enclosure size for adult mice is 330 cm2, with the minimum floor area per mouse, under group housing conditions, ranging from 60 to 100 cm2 depending on body weight. The minimum permitted cage height is 12 cm. The most common types of cages are conventional open-top cages (Figure 3a) and individually ventilated cages (IVCs) (Figure 3b).

A conventional open-top cage used to house mice in laboratory settings. Features a plastic box topped with a metal grate with a side hopper to hold food and water. A black mouse can be seen through the metal grate.
Figure 3a. Conventional open-top cage.
Conventional IVC (Individually Ventilated Cage). The cage has both a plastic top and bottom to allow air to be circulated in and out of the cage. Two white mice are visible through the side of the cage.
Figure 3b. IVC (Individually Ventilated Cage) – image courtesy of King's College London

Care should be taken to arrange the living area in a way that allows mice to compartmentalise their space. For example, water and food should be provided in one end of the cage leaving the other end free to be used for urination and defecation [16]. Tubes, boxes and climbing structures can be provided in order to increase cage complexity and the available floor area and allow spatial compartmentalisation. Mice prefer solid floors with suitable bedding, such as wood shavings [19]. Use of grid or wire mesh floors requires strong scientific justification and should be avoided as this can lead to injuries. If grids or wire mesh are used, a solid or bedded area should be provided for the animals to rest on unless specific experimental conditions prevent this and are authorised by the project licence [20].

The living space of mice is three dimensional and consists of the floor area as well as the vertical space. Climbing is an important locomotor activity in mice, which can be facilitated by incorporating a grid section in the cage lid. Grid space is limited to the food hopper in IVCs; however additional climbing devices, such as mouse lofts and mezzanines (Figure 4), can be provided.

A mouse mezzanine in an IVC. The mezzanine is made from red plastic which provides a dark shelter as the mice are unable to see in the red light spectrum, whilst allowing technicians and researchers to see the animals. One black mouse can be seen under the mezzanine in among bedding materials, another is climbing on top of the mezzanine demonstrating how the enrichment can provide more climbing space. A cardboard tunnel can be seen in the corner of the cage as evidence of further enrichment.
Figure 4. A mouse mezzanine can be included in IVCs to provide more climbing space, shelter, and protection from flooding where automatic watering systems are used. Image courtesy of University College London.

Social housing

Mice are highly social animals and in the wild they live in family groups usually consisting of a dominant male, several females with their offspring, and subordinate males [15]. As this arrangement is often not feasible in the laboratory, same sex group housing of mice can be implemented instead in order to facilitate their need for social interaction. Group-housing also promotes social exploration, and the behavioural activities of one animal—such as scent marking or digging—may also be a valuable source of novelty that elicits exploration by other group members [12] (Figure 5). More importantly, group-housed mice provide each other with social support (“social buffering”) when faced with a stressful situation [21]. For example, a study by Pham and colleagues found that the social environment influences the need for pain relief during post-operative recovery, with individually-housed mice self-administering higher levels of analgesia following a surgical procedure than socially-housed mice [22].

Not all members of a group are necessarily socially compatible and group housing of incompatible mice can lead to aggression, chronic stress, injury and occasionally death. Episodes of aggression are more common in group-housed male than female mice, as male mice tend to be more territorial. A crowdsourcing study conducted by the NC3Rs, analysed data on 137,580 mice from 44 facilities and found that the mean facility-level prevalence of aggression was 15 in 1,000 mice, with some strains showing higher prevalence than others [11]. The authors give several recommendations for minimising aggression.

Where aggression does occur, this can lead to single housing and social isolation of mice. However, a number of strategies can be adopted to reduce single housing, aside from taking steps to minimise aggression [23]. The ‘companion mouse’ strategy is good for transgenic strains when only one mouse in a cage has the desired phenotype. As adult male mice cannot be grouped with unfamiliar males of the same strain, keeping a ‘companion’ littermate that doesn’t carry the gene of interest can help avoid the need for single housing. An alternative solution is the post-weaning grouping strategy that consists of incorporating the single male into a cage of newly-weaned males with less than one week age difference. In general, it is advised to keep adult mice in stable groups and avoid constantly removing and introducing new individuals as this could destabilise the social hierarchy already established [11,24].

Mice heavily rely on olfactory cues in order to recognise their environment and communicate with their cage-mates [25,26]. Cage cleaning removes these scent marks [28], and can disrupt the social hierarchy and lead to increased levels of aggression [29]. Gerdin et al. (2012) showed that cage change can also lead to a significant increase in systolic arterial pressure, heart rate and locomotor activity, with these effects lasting for more than one hour [25]. These observations have led to an increasing number of husbandry recommendations that support reduced frequency of cage cleaning, spot cleaning of the dirty bedding/substrate as needed (instead of changing the whole cage), and the transfer of clean and dry nesting material during cage change in order to maintain familiar odours in the new cage [11,29,30]. Aggression-reducing scent marks, that primarily originate from saliva, plantar sweat, and urine [27], are carried in the nesting material.

The social environment is also important for pup survival and wellbeing. Reproductive asynchrony – the presence of two litters in a single cage with a large age difference between them (Figure 6) – has been identified as a key factor for high pup mortality [31]. Therefore, care should be taken to avoid housing two or more litters of different ages together, potentially by removing and temporarily single housing the pregnant females prior to parturition or opting to set up breeding pairs instead of breeding trios.

Lights and sounds

Mouse rooms typically have a 12-hour light-dark cycle. It is recommended that light levels within the enclosure are low, particularly for albino animals as they are more susceptible to light-induced retinal damage [32]. Racks should ideally have shaded tops in order to keep light intensity to a minimum and reduce the risk of retinal degeneration [20].

Mice are very sensitive to ultrasound, which they use for communication. Sources of ultrasound in laboratories include dripping taps, trolley wheels and computer monitors, which may affect mouse behaviour and breeding cycles [21,33]. As some sources of ultrasound cannot be eliminated (e.g. laboratory equipment), playing background music may help mask stressful sounds [15]. It is advisable to minimise sudden irregular noises as they create more disturbance in breeding rodents than continuous or predictable sounds and can increase the risk of mis-mothering or cannibalism [20].

Temperature

According to UK Home Office guidelines, the room temperature for mice should be 20–24°C (68–75°F) and kept as stable as possible [20]. However, the thermoneutral zone of mice is between 26°C and 34°C (79–93°F) and therefore they may experience cold stress in the laboratory unless nesting material is provided for them to regulate their body temperature and the microclimate within the cage. Sufficient nesting material should be provided that enables them to create good nests, which can reach temperatures of 30–32°C (86–90°F) [34,35]. This is particularly crucial for newborn pups which lack the ability to thermoregulate and therefore their survival depends on the warmth of their mother and siblings, which is retained by the nesting material [36] (Figure 7).

Ventilation and humidity

The air in the mouse room should be renewed at frequent intervals. Typically, a ventilation rate of five to 20 air changes per hour is adequate for a fully stocked room of mice. The relative humidity should be kept at 45 to 65% [20].

For IVCs (Figure 8), the number of air changes per hour typically ranges between 25 and 120. As air is blown into the cage at relatively high speed, the intra-cage ventilation rate could lead to chronic stress as well as heat loss due to the draught [37]. Therefore, mice in IVCs should be monitored for any signs of anxiety or discomfort, and the air change rate should be adjusted accordingly. Signs that the mice are reacting to the high air flow include change of location of the nest or building protective barriers against the draught using bedding material [16]. If such changes in behaviour are detected care should be taken to decrease the intra-cage draughts and provide mice a comfortable environment to live in.

Environmental enrichment

Environmental enrichment (Figure 9) provides sensory and motor stimulation for captive animals and allows them a greater choice of activity and some control over their environment [13,38]. Bedding and nesting material, refuges, and gnawing sticks are the most commonly used enrichment resources for mice and should normally be provided in every mouse cage unless there is sound scientific justification not to. Giving mice the opportunity to perform species-typical behaviours, such as nest building or foraging, can significantly improve their lives in the laboratory. Gnawing is also an important behaviour for mouse welfare; as their teeth grow continually through their lives, an inability to gnaw can lead to malocclusion (overgrowth of incisors) and consequently malnourishment. Barren enclosures are associated with signs of impaired welfare, such as anxiety and abnormal repetitive behaviour [13]. During breeding, lack of enrichment has been shown to result in higher pup mortality, reduced pup weight, and delayed development [36].

Mice prefer nesting material over every other form of enrichment [39], including prefabricated refuges, manipulanda and social contact [9]. Nest building is well preserved from wild‐type progenitor mice and is undertaken equally by female and male mice. Assessing the quality of the nest is a great cage-side tool to identify health and well-being issues (such as thermal stress, aggressiveness, sickness, and pain) and nest scoring protocols are available in the literature [40,42,43,44]. Even mice at the preclinical stage of a disease (i.e. not yet showing any clear clinical symptoms) may fail to build adequate nests, if they manage to build a nest at all [40,43].

Common nesting materials for laboratory mice include facial tissue, shredded paper strips, compressed cotton squares (nestlets), wood wool, and wood shavings [45,46]. Mice seem to prefer paper-derived materials (e.g. shredded paper) over wood-derived materials (e.g. cotton squares), however, preference for a particular nesting material might depend on the structure rather than the nature (paper or wood) which determines how easily they can manipulated to create a nest [47]. Providing appropriate nesting material can allow even “nest-building challenged” strains (e.g. C3H mice) to build nests comparable to stains with strong building skills (e.g. BALB/c) [48]. Additionally, care should be taken when choosing nesting material for some groups of mice, such as breeding mice or post-surgery mice, as some types of nesting material might have a negative effect on animal welfare [46]. For example, pups and adult mice could get entangled and injured in cotton wool [49,50], and mice with head implants risk getting tangled while trying to build a nest using facial tissue [51] (Figure 10).

Mice also show preferences for different bedding materials, shelters and gnawing resources, which can be sex or strain specific [20,52,53,54]. Therefore, provision of environmental enrichment shouldn’t be a random process whereby different objects are included in a mouse cage based merely upon their availability; instead care should be taken to assess the true beneficial effect on the animals’ overall well-being [15,55].

Summary of the key features of mouse housing and husbandry

  1. Cages should be large enough to provide enough space for exercise and normal social behaviour, with a minimum height of 12 cm.
  2. Cages should be well ventilated, softly lit and subject to a light dark cycle. The relative humidity should be kept at 45 to 65%.
  3. If mouse rooms and cages are kept at the recommended temperature range of 20-24oC, sufficient nesting material should be provided so the mice are able to regulate their body temperature and microclimate within the cage and avoid experiencing cold stress.
  4. Extraneous noise, and in particular ultrasound, should be kept to the minimum.
  5. Mice should be socially-housed in stable groups with compatible cage mates.
  6. Cage cleaning frequency should be kept to the minimum and nesting material should be transferred to clean cages to maintain familiar odours and minimize aggression.
  7. Cages should have solid floors covered with an adequate depth of appropriate substrate for hygiene, comfort and to permit natural foraging and digging behaviour.
  8. At least one shelter (e.g. cardboard tube or plastic house) should be provided to encourage exploration, facilitate resting and provide refuge, and the ability to manage social interactions.
  9. Appropriate and sufficient nesting and bedding material (e.g. 1 cm depth) should be provided.
  10. Gnawing material should be provided to avoid overgrown teeth which can result in malnourishment.
  11. The living area should be arranged in a way that allows mice to compartmentalise their space. Vertical barriers or tubes (e.g. PVC, aspen wood or cardboard) can add cage complexity, tactile comfort, escape routes, and exploration.
  12. Grid space or other climbing devices, such as mouse lofts and mezzanines, should be provided in order to facilitate climbing.

References

  1. Reynolds RP et al. (2010). Noise in a laboratory animal facility from the human and mouse perspectives. Journal of the American Association for Laboratory Animal Science 49(5): 592-597. PMID: 20858361
  2. Portfors CV (2007). Types and functions of ultrasonic vocalizations in laboratory rats and mice. Journal of the American Association for Laboratory Animal Science 46(1): 28-34. PMID: 17203913
  3. Matsumoto YK and Okanoya K (2018). Mice modulate ultrasonic calling bouts according to sociosexual context. Royal Society Open Science 5: 180378. doi: 10.1098/rsos.180378
  4. Van Segbroeck M et al. (2017). MUPET—Mouse Ultrasonic Profile ExTraction: A signal processing tool for rapid and unsupervised analysis of ultrasonic vocalizations. Neuron 94(3): 465-485. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.04.005
  5. Vogel AP et al. (2019). Quantifying ultrasonic mouse vocalizations using acoustic analysis in a supervised statistical machine learning framework. Scientific Reports 9: 8100. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-44221-3
  6. Crawley N et al. (1997). Behavioral phenotypes of inbred mouse strains: implications and recommendations for molecular studies. Psychopharmacology 132: 107-124. doi: 10.1007/s002130050327
  7. Lee C and Wong PTP (2013). Temperature effect and strain differences in the nest-building behavior of inbred mice. Animal Psychology 20: 9-10. doi: 10.3758/BF03335572
  8. Gaskill BN et al. (2013). Energy reallocation to breeding performance through improved nest building in laboratory mice. PloS ONE 8(9): e74153. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0074153
  9. Hutchinson E et al. (2005). Environmental enrichment for laboratory rodents. Institute of Laboratory Animal Research Journal 46(2): 148-61. doi: 10.1093/ilar.46.2.162
  10. van de Weerd HA et al. (1994). Strain specific behavioural response to environmental enrichment in the mouse. Journal of Experimental Animal Science 36(4-5): 117-127. PMID: 7948063
  11. Lidster K et al. (2019). Cage aggression in group-housed laboratory male mice: an international data crowdsourcing project. Scientific Reports 9(1): 15211. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-51674-z
  12. Olsson IAS and Westlund K (2007). More than numbers matter: The effect of social factors on behaviour and welfare of laboratory rodents and non-human primates. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 103(3-7): 229-254. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2006.05.022
  13. Bailoo JD et al. (2018). Effects of cage enrichment on behavior, welfare and outcome variability in female mice. Frontiers in Behavioural Neuroscience 12: 232. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00232
  14. Kappel S et al. (2017). To group or not to group? Good practice for housing male laboratory mice. Animals 7(12). pii: E88. doi: 10.3390/ani7120088
  15. van Loo PLP and Baumans V (2015). Mice. In: Comfortable Quarters for Laboratory Animals (Eds. Liss C, Litwak K, Tilford D, Reinhardt V), Animal Welfare Institute.
  16. Makowska J et al. (2019). Standard laboratory housing for mice restricts their ability to segregate space into clean and dirty areas. Scientific Reports 9(1): 6179. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-42512-3
  17. Sherwin CM andGlen EF (2003). Cage colour preferences and effects of home cage colour on anxiety in laboratory mice. Animal Behaviour 66(6): 1085-1092. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2003.2286
  18. Porter G et al. (1963). A comparison between transparent and opaque cages for breeding mice. Journal of the Animal Technicians Association (13): 84.
  19. Jackson E et al. (2015). Aspen shaving versus chip bedding: effects on breeding and behavior. Laboratory Animals 49(1): 46-56. doi: 10.1177/0023677214553320
  20. Home Office (2014). Mice, rats, gerbils, hamsters and guinea pigs. In: Code of Practice for the Housing and Care of Animals Bred, Supplied or Used for Scientific Purposes. Her Majesty’s Stationery Office.
  21. Hennessy MB et al. (2009). Social buffering of the stress response: Diversity, mechanisms, and functions. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 30(4): 470-482. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2009.06.001
  22. Pham TM et al. (2010). Housing environment influences the need for pain relief during post-operative recovery in mice. Physiology and Behaviour 99(5): 663-8. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.01.038
  23. Azkona G and Caballero JM (2019). Implementing strategies to reduce singly housed male mice. Laboratory Animals 53(5): 508–510. doi: 10.1177/0023677219845028
  24. Gaskill BN et al. (2019). The effect of early life experience, environment, and genetic factors on spontaneous home-cage aggression-related wounding in male C57BL/6 mice. Lab Animal 46(4): 176-184. doi: 10.1038/laban.1225
  25. Gerdin A et al. (2012). Experimental and husbandry procedures as potential modifiers of the results of phenotyping tests. Physiology and Behavior 106(5): 602-11. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.03.026
  26. Burn CC et al. (2006) Long-term effects of cage-cleaning frequency and bedding type on laboratory rat health, welfare, and handleability: a cross-laboratory study. Laboratory Animals 40(4): 353-70. doi: 10.1258/002367706778476460
  27. Barabas AJ et al. (2019). Proteome characterization of used nesting material and potential protein sources from group housed male mice, Mus musculus. Scientific Reports 9: 17524. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-53903-x
  28. Hurst JL (1993). The priming effects of urine substrate marks on interactions between male house mice, Mus musculus domesticus Schwarz and Schwarz. Animal Behaviour 45(1): 55-81. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1993.1007
  29. Van Loo PLP et al. (2000). Modulation of aggression in male mice: influence of cage cleaning regime and scent marks. Animal Welfare 9(3): 281-295. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(01)00425-5
  30. Van Loo PLP et al. (2003). Male management: coping with aggression problems in male laboratory mice. Laboratory Animals 37(4): 300-13. doi: 10.1258/002367703322389870
  31. Brajona S et al. (2019). Social environment as a cause of litter loss in laboratory mouse: A behavioural study. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 218: 104827. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2019.06.008
  32. Peirson SN et al. (2018). Light and the laboratory mouse. Journal of Neuroscience Methods 300: 26-36. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2017.04.007
  33. Sales GD et al. (1988). Environmental ultrasound in laboratories and animal houses: A possible cause for concern in the welfare and use of laboratory animals. Laboratory Animals 22(4): 369-75. doi: 10.1258/002367788780746188
  34. Gaskill BN et al. (2011). Working with what you’ve got: Changes in thermal preference and behavior in mice with or without nesting material. Journal of Thermal Biology 36(3): 193–199. doi: 10.1016/j.jtherbio.2011.02.004
  35. Gaskill BN et al. (2013). Impact of nesting material on mouse body temperature and physiology. Physiology and Behavior 110–111: 87-95. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.12.018
  36. Leidinger CS et al. (2019). Environmental enrichment prevents pup mortality in laboratory mice. Laboratory Animals 53(1): 53-62. doi: 10.1177/0023677218777536
  37. Baumans V and Van Loo PLP (2013). How to improve housing conditions of laboratory animals: The possibilitiesof environmental refinement. Veterinary Journal 195(1): 24-32. doi: 10.1016/j.tvjl.2012.09.023
  38. Baumans V (2005). Environmental enrichment for laboratory rodents and rabbits: requirements of rodents, rabbits, and research. Institute of Laboratory Animal Research Journal 46(2): 162-70. doi: 10.1093/ilar.46.2.162
  39. Olsson IA and Dahlborn K (2002). Improving housing conditions for laboratory mice: a review of ‘environmental enrichment’. Laboratory animals 36(3): 243-70. doi: 10.1258/002367702320162379
  40. Deacon R (2012). Assessing burrowing, nest construction, and hoarding in mice. Journal of Visualized Experiments (59): e2607. doi: 10.3791/2607
  41. Jirkof P (2013). Assessment of postsurgical distress and pain in laboratory mice by nest complexity scoring. Lab Animals 47(3): 153-61. doi: 10.1177/0023677213475603
  42. Kraeuter A et al. (2019). The nest building test in mice for assessment of general welleing. Methods in Molecular Biology. 1916: 87-91. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4939-8994-2_7
  43. Gaskill BN et al. (2013). Nest building as an indicator of health and welfare in laboratory mice. Journal of Visualized Experiments (82): 51012. doi: 10.3791/51012
  44. Jirkof P (2010). Burrowing Behavior as an Indicator of Post-Laparotomy Pain in Mice. Frontiers in Behavioural Neuroscience 4: 165. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2010.00165
  45. Hess SE et al. (2008). Home improvement: C57BL/6J mice given more naturalistic nesting materials build better nests. Journal of the American Association for Laboratory Animal Science 47(6): 25-31. PMCID: PMC2687128
  46. Bayne K (2018). Environmental enrichment and mouse models: Current perspectives. Animal Models and Experimental Medicine 1(2): 82-90. doi: 10.1002/ame2.12015
  47. Van de Weerd HA et al. (1997). Preferences for nesting material as environmental enrichment for laboratory mice. Laboratory Animals 31(2): 133-43. doi: 10.1258/002367797780600152
  48. Robinson-Junker A et al. (2017). Sorting it out: bedding particle size and nesting material processing method affect nest complexity. Laboratory Animals 51(2): 170-180. doi: 10.1177/0023677216652384
  49. Rowson KE and Michaels L (1980). Injury to young mice caused by cottonwool used as nesting material. Laboratory Animals 14(3): 187. doi: 10.1258/002367780780937562
  50. Northrup E et al. (2012). Mice may become caught in nesting material. Lab Animal 41: 147. doi: 10.1038/laban0612-147
  51. Windsor Z and Bate ST (2019). Assessing the safety and suitability of nesting material for singly housed mice with surgically fitted head plates. Heliyon 5(7): e02097. doi: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2019.e02097
  52. Kirchner J et al. (2012). Preferences of group-housed female mice regarding structure of softwood bedding. Laboratory Animals 46(2): 95-100. doi: 10.1258/la.2011.010173
  53. Lopez Juaristi I (2019). A comparison of enrichment items for the promotion of natural gnawing behaviour in laboratory miceAnimal Technology and Welfare 18(2): 93-97.
  54. Van Loo PL et al. (2005). Assessment of the use of two commercially available environmental enrichments by laboratory mice by preference testing. Laboratory Animals 39(1): 58-67. doi: 10.1258/0023677052886501
  55. Russel WMS and Burch RL (1959). The principles of humane experimental technique. Universities Federation For Animal Welfare (UFAW), London.

Housing and husbandry in other species